Introduction

Intimate partner violence, particularly against women, is a pervasive worldwide phenomenon with serious societal and public health consequences for individuals, families, communities and societies.1, 2 Despite increasing efforts to develop prevention and intervention programs that address intimate partner violence, evidence of positive outcomes is scarce because of an insufficient understanding of its causes.1, 3 Although the roots of partner violence may be complex, childhood exposure to violence, either as a victim or a witness, has been identified as a major risk factor for intimate violence perpetration.4, 5 To date, the prevailing etiological theories proposed to explain intimate partner violence have emphasized two prominent factors: (i) the cultural environment, particularly the impact of male dominance in society (Cultural theory), or (ii) social learning, whereby children learn patterns of violence by watching relevant early figures—parents in particular—that are reproduced in intimate partner relationships in adulthood, which enables violence to be transmitted from one generation to the next (Social learning theory). Despite increasing evidence that early exposure to stress can affect the neurodevelopmental trajectories that influence an individual's behavior in adulthood,6, 7, 8 whether biological factors can account for the intergenerational transmission of intimate partner violence, both within an individual lifetime and across generations, remains under-researched.9 Most of the studies directed at identifying risk factors at the biological level are cross-sectional studies focused mainly on demographics, live stressful experiences,10, 11 mental disorders12, 13 and substance abuse,14, 15 remaining unclear whether those biological factors are the cause or correlated factors of the violence perpetrated by the abusive partner.1, 9

To seek evidence for the contribution of biological factors to the intergenerational transmission of intimate partner violence, we selected an animal model (that is, devoid of human cultural connotations) whereby exposing male rats to fear-inducing experiences from juvenility to puberty (that is, the peripubertal period ranging from postnatal days P28–P42; subsequently referred to as peripubertally stressed rats) was shown to produce pathological aggression against males during adulthood.16 In this model, fear was induced in a non-social context, which allowed the exclusion of potential vicarious social learning. To determine whether peripubertal stress would result in abnormal aggressive behavior against a female partner, males from both the stress and the control groups were individually paired with a female and left to cohabit in a home cage for 21 days. Furthermore, we sought to evaluate whether biological factors could also account for the transfer of a behavioral profile of pathological aggression to an intimate partner across generations. To this end, we investigated whether the male offspring of peripubertally stressed males displayed increased aggression toward a female partner.

Materials and methods

Animals

The male experimental subjects were the offspring (F0) of Wistar Han rats purchased from Charles River Laboratories (Lyon, France) that were bred in our animal house, as well as the offspring (F1) from F0 male and F0 females. The female experimental subjects were virgin Wistar rats acquired at 10-week-old from Charles River Laboratories. Animals were maintained under controlled conditions (12 h light/dark cycle; lights on at 0700 hours; 22±2 °C). Food and water were available ad libitum. With the exception of animals submitted to peripubertal stress (see procedure below), all animals were at least 3-month-old at the beginning of the experiments. With the exception of the home cage interactions and maternal behavior observations (see details below), animal testing occurred during the first half of the animals’ light phase. All conducted procedures conformed to Swiss National Institutional Guidelines on Animal Experimentation and were approved through a license by the Swiss Cantonal Veterinary Office Committee for Animal Experimentation.

F0 male rats

F0 male subjects were the male offspring of rats obtained from Charles River Laboratories. Male offspring from different litters were weaned at postnatal day 21. Rats from different litters were mixed and housed with 2–3 animals per standard plastic cage; an equivalent number of animals from each litter were randomly placed in the peripubertal stress and control groups (N=22 per group).

Peripubertal stress applied to F0 males

We followed a previously described peripubertal stress protocol.16, 17 This protocol involves two different fear-inducing stressors: (i) the synthetic fox odor trimethylthiazoline (Phero Tech, Delta, BC, Canada) is a synthetic version of a compound found in fox feces that evokes innate fear in rats. A small amount of trimethylthiazoline (9 μl) was absorbed onto a small cloth and administered in a white plastic box (38 × 27.5 × 31 cm) placed under a bright light (200–250 lux) and (ii) exposure to an elevated platform (12 × 12 cm, elevated 95 cm from the ground) under direct bright light (500–550 lux). This is a stressful experience for rats because of their innate fear of height and open spaces. Each of these stressors lasted 25 min and was applied in a semi-random order. Following each stress session, the animals were returned to their home cages. During the first 15 min, a transparent Plexiglas (MSPLAST, Pampigny, Switzerland) wall with holes separated each of the animals in the cage. The stressors were applied during the peripuberty period on an unpredictable schedule (a total of 7 days across P28–P42; on P28–30, P34, P36 and P40–P42) during the light phase. Stressor order and timing were changed on different days. On some stress days, only one stressor was presented; on others both were given consecutively. The control animals were handled on the days that their experimental counterparts were exposed to stress.

F0 female partners

Adult (10-week-old) virgin female Wistar rats (Charles River Laboratories) were used as experimental subjects. Upon arrival, female rats were housed in groups of three per cage (see experimental protocol in Figure 1a). Two weeks later, they were caged with a 3-month-old F0 adult male (either control or peripubertally stressed, N=22 per gender per group) for 21 days. At the end of this period, the male was removed before parturition and tissue paper was provided to facilitate nest building. Dams and offspring were not disturbed until P7, when the first cage change after delivery was performed. With the exception of weekly cage changing, litters and dams remained undisturbed until weaning on P21. Two days later, the females were again housed in groups of three (with their original cagemates) and kept under these housing conditions during the subsequent behavioral testing phase. One week after female re-housing, all females were submitted to a battery of behavioral tests that lasted 6–7 weeks (Figure 1b; control N=10; stress, N=7).

Figure 1
figure 1

Schematic of the experimental procedures, including the general experimental design (a) and the sequence of behavioral tests performed in the females (b). Abbreviations: d, days; P, postnatal days.

F1 male rats

Weaned male offspring of F0 males and females were reared in groups of three of same sex. Rats from different litters within the same experimental group (F1 control N=13; F1 stress N=11) were mixed in the respective home cages. Animals were left undisturbed until adulthood (three-month-old).

F1 female partners

The females that were caged with F1 male rats at the age of 3 months were the offspring of F0 male and F0 female controls. Once weaned, these females were reared in groups of 3 rats per cage. Rats from different litters were mixed. They were left undisturbed until adulthood (3-month-old), at which time they were individually caged with an F1 adult male (either control or the offspring of stressed F0 male rats, N=11–13 per gender per group) for 21 days. All other procedures were the same as described above for F0 female partners. Behavioral testing was carried out in N=13 controls and N=11 F1 female stress rats.

Behavioral observations during male–female cohabitation

During male–female cohabitation, the home cage was changed three times (once per week) at 1700–1900 hours. This arousing experience stimulates social interactions, and for this reason it was used as the starting time for behavioral observations. The first time each male and female were put together in the home cage, and the periods immediately following home cage changing on days 7 and 14 were video-recorded for 30 min. The number of attacks from the male to the female and the time during which the female displayed defensive-submissive behavior (either freezing or being in a supine position under the male) were scored by an experimenter who was blinded to the treatment conditions and assisted by a computer program (The Observer 5.0.25, Noldus, Wageningen, The Netherlands, 2003). Animals were not visibly wounded by these behaviors, except for superficial scars on a few females.

Observations of maternal behavior

Maternal care for the first 6 days postpartum for each dam was observed using a protocol adapted from Champagne et al.18 Detailed methods are provided in the Supplementary Material.

Behavioral characterization of female emotionality

The long-term emotional effects of 21-day cohabitation with male partners were assessed 10 days after weaning using a battery of tests in the following order: elevated plus maze, open field (OF) and novel object reactivity test, forced swim test (FST), startle response, prepulse inhibition (PPI) and fear induction experiences (shock-induced in F0 and male-induced in F1). There were at least 7 days between each test, with the total emotional testing period lasting 6–7 weeks. All behavioral testing was conducted between 0900 and 1400 hours. Except for the fear induction, shock-induced and the startle and PPI that were recorded automatically by software programs (see below), behavior was monitored using a video camera located on the ceiling, and movements of the rats were automatically registered and analyzed with a computerized tracking system (Ethovision 3.1.16, Noldus IT). Detailed methods are provided in the Supplementary Material.

Exposure to unfamiliar male as a challenge for neurobiological analyses

All F1 female partners reported in the F1 study and a group of F0 female partners were generated under equal conditions as reported for the F0 study, except for the last behavioral test of fear induction to shock. In order to avoid potential interference of shock-induced learned helplessness with the neurobiological mechanisms investigated (see below19), the females of this study were exposed to a naïve unfamiliar male while in diestrus phase and before killing. For this test, each group of females was randomly divided into two conditions: basal (home cage control condition, N=4–5 per group) or exposure to an unfamiliar male (male exposure condition, N=6–8 per group). In the male exposure condition, the females were placed in a fresh cage containing a transparent Plexiglas separator that divided the cage into two compartments (20 L × 12 W × 35 H cm). The small size of these compartments ensured proximity and exposure of the experimental animal to the male stimulus. After a 5-min habituation period, an unknown sexually inexperienced 3-month-old male was placed in the other compartment. The separator contained many small holes allowing visual, auditory and olfactory stimuli to pass between the chambers. After 15 min, the females were returned to their home cage until they were killed (see details below in Brain processing and sectioning section).

Plasma corticosterone

To assess possible alterations of corticosterone circadian rhythm as a consequence of male–female cohabitation with different types of males, blood samples were obtained from all females at two time points (morning, between 0800 and 0900 hours; and night, between 2000 and 2100 hours), 1 week after the FST (see experimental schedule in Figure 1b). Blood samples were collected by tail nick, which consisted of gently wrapping the animals with a cloth, making a 2-mm incision at the end of one of the tail arteries and then massaging the tail while collecting, within 2 min, 300 μl of blood into ice-cold, heparin capillary tubes (Microvett cb300, Sarstedt, Sevelen, Switzerland). The tubes were centrifuged at 4 °C (4500 r.p.m., 20 min), and plasma aliquots were stored at −20 °C until the assay was performed. Because samples were collected on the same day (morning and night), the second collection was obtained by gently removing the scab from the first incision.

Determination of estrous cycle phase

The estrous cycle was monitored in all females during the 3 weeks before the unfamiliar male challenge for neurobiological analyses (see above). Vaginal swabs were taken daily in all rats. Female rats were lightly restrained manually and a swab wet with room temperature normal saline solution (NaCl 0.9%) was gently and shallowly inserted into the vagina and smeared on a new slide. The slide was immediately fixed with 1% toluidine blue solution and observed under the microscope ( × 10 and × 40 objective lenses) to evaluate cells that characterize each stage of the estrous cycle (for details on the procedure, see Mandl20).

Tissue processing for immunohistochemistry

Brain processing and sectioning

Two hours after onset of exposure to the male stimulus (see Exposure to unfamiliar male as a challenge for neurobiological analyses) or at an equivalent time for the home cage group taken directly from their home cages, the females were anesthetized with intraperitoneal injections of sodium pentobarbital and intracardially perfused with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS, 0.1 M) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS (pH 7.4) before brain removal. Whole brains were incubated in 30% sucrose until saturation and then flash-frozen in isopentane on dry ice and kept at −80 °C until processing. A cryostat (Leica, CM 3050 S, Heerbrugg, Switzerland) was used to cut 30-μm sections that were stored as six alternate sets. Slices were been kept in cryoprotectant solution (glycerol, ethylene glycol in PBS) at −20 °C until immunohistochemistry was performed.

Double-immunostaining of c-Fos and serotonin (5-HT)

A set of brain sections (usually a dozen per subject) including dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN; from −7.30 mm to −9.30 mm from the bregma21 was processed sequentially for immunohistochemical labeling of c-Fos and 5-HT. All tissues were processed with same solutions. Although multiple baskets were necessary to process all the slices, tissue from different groups was counterbalanced among the baskets to reduce potential immunohistochemistry lot artifacts. Sections were washed (4 × 10 min in PBS, pH 7.4), and then endogenous peroxidase was blocked by incubation in a solution consisting of 1 volume of 30% H2O2, 1 volume of methanol, and 8 volumes of 0.1 M PBS with Triton-X100 (PBST; Sigma-Aldrich, Buchs SG, Switzerland) for 10 min and then washed again. Next, the sections were incubated for 60 min in blocking buffer (5% skim milk, 5% normal serum and 0.01% sodium azide in PBST), followed by a wash before incubation in anti-c-Fos antibody in PBST (1:6000, sc-52, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Heidelberg, Germany) for 48 h at 4 °C. Sections were then washed again before incubation in secondary antibody with normal serum for 2 h at room temperature (Elite Vector Kit, rabbit IgG, Vector Labs, Peterborough, UK). After another wash, brains slices were incubated for 1 h with avidin-biotin-peroxidase complex (Elite ABC Vector kit, Vector Labs) at room temperature. Sections were washed once more and finally rinsed in 0.05 M Tris buffer (pH 7.4). Final staining was obtained using 3,3′-diaminobenzidine (DAB) chromogen with nickel enhancement (as per manufacturer's instructions; DAB substrate kit, Vector Labs) and washing afterwards with cold PBS to stop the reaction. The sections were then labeled for 5-HT in a similar fashion but without the blocking steps. The primary antibody solution used consisted of anti-5HT antibody in PBST (1:2000, S5545, Sigma-Aldrich), which was visualized with SG chromogen (SG Substrate Kit, Vector Labs). The sections were mounted on microscope slides, dehydrated with a series of graded alcohols, cleared with xylene and coverslipped.

Double-labeled cell counting

Sections were viewed with an Olympus BX51 microscope (Olympus, Lausanne, Switzerland), the DRN and its subregions were identified,21 and double-labeled cells for c-Fos and 5-HT were assessed by an experimenter blind to the treatment groups. Images were acquired using a ColorView Camera ( × 10) (Olympus) and stored digitally. The cell counts were normalized to the area size.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS version 14.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). Normality and homogeneity of variance were tested and adjusted statistics were used if required. Parametric statistics were applied when data were normally distributed according to the Shapiro–Wilk test. Mean comparisons were carried out with either Student's t-tests or analyses of variance (either one-way or factorial analyses of variances with or without repeated measures) as appropriate. Sphericity assumed modeling, with Greenhouse–Geisser (ɛ<0.075) and Huynh–Feldt (ɛ>0.075) adjustments, was applied. Simple main effect analyses and post hoc comparisons were made with Fisher's least significant difference test, as appropriate. When the data were not normally distributed even after square root or log 10 transformations, non-parametric analyses were carried out with the Mann–Whitney tests. If Levene's test for equality of variances was significant, equal variances were not assumed, and the altered degrees of freedom were rounded to the nearest whole number. Correlational analyses between the psychobiological measures obtained in the females were performed using the Pearson's correlation test. Values in graphs represent the mean±s.e.m. Sample sizes are indicated in figure legends. The results were considered statistically significant if P<0.05 (*), 0.01 (**) or 0.001 (***).

Results

Effects of peripuberty stress on males’ aggressive behavior against females at adulthood

Analyses of male–female interactions indicated that peripubertally stressed males were more aggressive with females than the control males (Figure 2a), whereas females cohabiting with peripubertally stressed males showed a higher defensive-submissive behavior than those living with the control males (Figure 2b; see Supplementary Tables S3–S5, for a detailed statistical information).

Figure 2
figure 2

Aggressive and defensive behaviors during male–female cohabitation. During the 21 days of male–female cohabitation between pairs of rats involving either control or peripubertally stressed males (F0; N=22 per group) or control or male offspring from peripubertally stressed males (F1; N=11–13 per group), interactions were recorded weekly for 30 min beginning on the first day. (a and b) Males’ aggressive behavior toward their female partners as the total average (left panel) and across weekly observations (right panel). The peripubertally stressed males (F0) and their male offspring (F1) show higher aggression levels than the control males. (c and d) Females’ defensive behavior toward their male partner as the total average (left panel) and across weekly observations (right panel). Females (female stress) cohabiting with either a peripubertally stressed male or a male offspring from stressed fathers displayed a higher defensive behavior than those paired with a control male (female control). The results are the mean±s.e.m. *P<0.05; **P<0.01 versus control group.

Physiological and behavioral impact of cohabitation with the peripubertally stressed males on the female rats

To understand the physiological and behavioral impact of cohabitation with the peripubertally stressed males on the female rats, we monitored the females’ body weights and indexes of activity in the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis as well as their behavioral responses to tests for anxiety- and depression-like behavior following cohabitation. After weaning, females were caged in groups of three from the same experimental group to avoid isolation stress while investigating the long-term impact of cohabitation with an aggressive male. Female partners of the peripubertally stressed males showed decreased body weights both during and up to 10 weeks after cohabitation (Supplementary Figure S1A, B, Supplementary Table S29). These females also showed an altered hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis function, as indicated by alterations in the circadian pattern of plasma corticosterone levels monitored 7 weeks after cohabitation (Figure 3a, Supplementary Table S10) and by enlarged adrenal glands relative to the control females (Supplementary Table S1). At the behavioral level, females formerly partnered with the peripubertally stressed males (F0) showed the following behaviors: (i) enhanced anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze (Figure 3b, Supplementary Table S11) and startle responses (Figure 3d, Supplementary Tables S19, S20); (ii) increased motor reactivity both in an OF and in response to a novel object (Figure 3c, Supplementary Tables S13, S15, S17, S18); (iii) increased PPI (Figure 3e, Supplementary Table S21); (iv) enhanced depression-like behavior in the FST (Figure 3f, Supplementary Table S22) and (v) enhanced fear responses to mild aversive stimulation (Figure 3g, Supplementary Table S23).

Figure 3
figure 3

Long-term physiological and behavioral impact of male–female cohabitation on females. (a) Circadian plasma corticosterone levels evaluated at 7 weeks after cohabitation. Females that had cohabited with either a peripubertally stressed male (F0) or a male offspring from stressed fathers (F1) had lower morning corticosterone levels than controls. In behavioral tests of emotionality, compared with the control females, rats in the Female stress groups showed the following behaviors: (b) increased anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze, spending more time in the closed arms and less in the center than the controls; (c) increased locomotion in the open field (OF) and novel object (NO) tests; (d) increased startle response, both as the average of the session and in the first pulse; (e) increased prepulse inhibition (PPI); (f) increased depression-like behavior, displaying increased time floating in the forced swimming test and (g) increased fear-related behavior, as indicated by their enhanced freezing after repeated exposure to mild aversive stimulation (a 0.2-mA footshock delivered every 2 min; F0) and by reduced exploration of an unfamiliar male when tested during estrus (F1). F0: N=7–10 per group; F1: N=11–13 per group. The results are the mean±s.e.m. *P<0.05, **P<0.01 versus control group.

Increased aggression toward females in the male offspring of peripubertally stressed males

We sought to evaluate whether biological factors could also account for the transfer of a behavioral profile of pathological aggression to an intimate partner across generations. To this end, we investigated whether the male offspring of peripubertally stressed males displayed increased aggression toward a female partner, similar to their fathers, and examined the physiological and behavioral consequences of this interaction for their female partners. Overall, the group of all female partners of peripubertally stressed males presented obstetric problems, cannibalism and altered maternal care (Figure 4, Supplementary Tables S2, S30). However, importantly, the violence transmission part of the study was performed in offspring whose maternal care did not seem to differ from the one displayed by control females [(Figure 4, Supplementary Figure S2, Supplementary Table S30); maternal behavior was scored in the dams’ home cage during the first 6 days postpartum according to Champagne et al.,18] as only the male offspring (two males per litter were selected for the follow-up study, representing the average body weight of the litter) of females not displaying obstetric problems or cannibalism were used for the F1 study. Thus, we pursued an experimental design equivalent to the design followed for the previous generation (that is 21 days of male–female cohabitation, with males from either control or peripubertally stressed fathers and females always from control rats, followed by a behavioral and physiological assessment of the females). Strikingly, the results showed that the male offspring, without postnatal contact with their fathers, reproduced the response pattern of their peripubertally stressed fathers. These rats were more aggressive toward female partners during cohabitation (Figure 2c, Supplementary Tables S6, S7) despite the female showing increased defensive-submissive behavior (Figure 2d, Supplementary Tables S8, S9).

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of F0 male–female cohabitation on female maternal behavior during the first 6 days postpartum. See Supplementary Material for descriptions of different types of maternal care measures. (ac) Analyses including all females in the F0 study indicated that females that cohabited with peripubertally stressed males show a reduced maternal care in certain measures. However, the significance of these tendencies was mainly because of three dams in the stress group that cannibalized their pups. N=10 per group. (df) Analyses performed excluding the data from the three dams that cannibalized their pups clearly indicate a lack of significant differences between the groups in any of the scored maternal behaviors. The offspring of these dams were included in the F1 study. Control, N=10; Stress, N=7. The results are the mean±s.e.m. (g) Percentage of cumulative time that stress and control dams spent engaging in each category of observed maternal behavior; representing raw data from panels af.

Physiological and behavioral impact of cohabitation with the male offspring of peripubertally stressed males on the female rats

As done with the peripubertally stressed males, we evaluated the impact of the interaction of their male offspring on their female partners after a 21-day cohabitation. Strikingly, the physiological [reduced body weight (Supplementary Figure S1, Supplementary Table S29); reduced plasma corticosterone morning levels (Figure 3a, Supplementary Table S10)] and behavioral [increased anxiety-like behavior and startle responses (Figures 3b and d, Supplementary Tables S12, S19), locomotor reactivity (Figure 3c, Supplementary Tables S14, S16–S18), PPI (Figure 3e, Supplementary Table S21), depression-like behavior (Figure 3f, Supplementary Table S22) and fear responses (Figure 3g, Supplementary Tables S23, S24)] impacts of cohabitation with the male offspring of peripubertally stressed males on the female partners was essentially a mirror of the alterations displayed by the female partners of the peripubertally stressed males (that is, their mothers).

In order to assess whether the endocrine and behavioral alterations observed in the abused females (from both F0 and F1) were mutually associated, we performed correlational analyses (see Supplementary Table S25 for controls and Supplementary Table S26 for stressed). Whereas some of the correlations were found both in the control and stressed groups (that is, a positive correlation between floating in the forced swim and distance in the OF), other were specifically observed in the stressed females (that is, a negative correlations between PPI and percent time in the closed arms, distance in the OF and floating in the forced swim). Remarkably, some of the correlations observed in control animals between morning corticosterone levels and some behavioral parameters (that is, negative correlations with PPI and locomotor reactivity to the novel object) were not found in the stressed group.

Neurobiological impact of cohabitation with the peripubertally stressed males or their male offspring on the female rats

To evaluate the reactivity of serotonergic neurons in the DRN in females previously living with an aggressive male (that is, either the peripubertally stressed males or their offspring), we exposed the females to an unfamiliar male for 15 min while in diestrus (that is, when female rats are more susceptible to aversive stimuli). Each male and female pair was confined in close proximity within the same fresh home cage. The pair was separated by a transparent plastic wall with holes that allowed smelling and observation but prevented direct physical contact. Two hours after this interaction, brains were collected to evaluate the activation of DRN 5-HT neurons (as assessed by immunohistochemical analyses of protein expression of the immediate early gene c-Fos in 5-HT-labeled neurons (Figure 5b, Supplementary Tables S27)). Additional groups of females for each experimental condition were killed in diestrus under basal conditions. When exposed to an unfamiliar male, the two groups of females that had previously lived with an aggressive male displayed a much greater (2.5-fold increase) c-Fos expression in DRN 5-HT neurons than the female rats that had previously been partnered with the control males (Figure 5, Supplementary Tables S28, S31, S32).

Figure 5
figure 5

Impact of male–female cohabitation on the percentage of dorsal raphe neurons double-labeled for serotonin (5-HT) and c-Fos. Ten weeks after the end of the male–female cohabitation, females were killed either under basal conditions (home cage; N=4–5 per group) or 2 h after being exposed for 15 min to an unfamiliar male (N=6–8 per group) while in diestrus. (a) Representative photomicrographs showing c-Fos (brown) and 5-HT (grey) immunoreactivity in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN), with inset depicting cells single-labeled c-Fos (black arrow), 5-HT (white arrow) or double-labeled (white arrow with black outline). Scale bar=200 μm, inset=20 μm. (b) When exposed to an unfamiliar male, the percentage of double-labeled 5-HT and c-Fos neurons was enhanced in the F0 and F1 female stress groups relative to the controls. Under basal conditions, the F1 female stress group showed a reduction in these double-labeled neurons. (c) Results in the dorsal DR (DRD); (d) results in the caudal DR (DRC); (e) Results in the ventral DR (DRV); (f): Results in the ventrolateral DR (DRVL). The results are the mean±s.e.m. *P<0.05 and **P<0.01 versus control group under the same conditions. +P<0.05 and ++P<0.01 versus the same group home cage.

Discussion

We investigated whether male's aggressive behavior toward female partners linked to an early traumatic stress could emerge independently of cultural and social learning factors. We also extended this question to the transmission of this aggressive behavior to the male offspring of the peripubertally stressed fathers. We used a rat model (that is, devoid of human cultural influences) to explore the possibility that biological factors could have an important role in these phenomena. Our results show that peripubertally stressed males (F0) displayed high aggression levels against their female partners. On its turn, female partners of the peripubertally stressed males showed a marked alteration in physiological (decreased body weight and basal plasma corticosterone) and behavioral responses (including increased anxiety- and depression-like behaviors). Strikingly, our results also showed that the male offspring (F1) reproduced the response pattern of their peripubertally stressed fathers. These rats were more aggressive toward female partners during cohabitation despite the female showing increased defensive-submissive behavior. Furthermore, the physiological and behavioral impact of cohabitation with the male offspring of peripubertally stressed males on the female partners was essentially a mirror of the alterations displayed by the female partners of the peripubertally stressed males (that is, their mothers).

In humans, surviving victims of domestic violence typically show long-term negative health consequences, notably including psychopathological alterations.22, 23 In our rat model, we show that the rat female partners of either peripubertally stressed males or their male offspring displayed decreased body weights both during and up to 10 weeks after cohabitation. These females also showed an altered hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis function, as indicated by alterations in the circadian pattern of plasma corticosterone levels monitored 7 weeks after cohabitation and by enlarged adrenal glands relative to the control females. At the behavioral level, they showed: (i) enhanced anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze and startle responses; (ii) increased motor reactivity both in an OF and in response to a novel object; (iii) increased PPI; (iv) enhanced depression-like behavior in the FST and (v) enhanced fear responses. Altogether, these results largely resemble the type of cortisol alterations24 and anxious and depressive behavior observed in abused women.25, 26, 27 Moreover, these symptoms match the alterations observed in women with posttraumatic stress disorder, a syndrome frequently described in abused women.27, 28 However, a note of caution should be added when considering the constellation of symptoms found in our rat study as a general syndrome. Correlational analyses indicated that the association between some behavioral parameters in stressed females show a similar pattern as in controls (for example, the higher the locomotor reactivity, the higher the floating in the FST). Importantly, they also revealed a specific pattern of associated comorbidity in the stressed females (for example, the higher the anxiety- and depression-like behaviors, the lower the PPI).

To further validate the trauma for the females of interacting with an aggressive male at a neurobiological level, we studied the activation of serotonergic neurons in the DRN. The DRN is a small nucleus located in the brainstem with primarily serotonergic neurons (5-HT). In association with the role of 5-HT in mood modulation,29 the serotonergic neurons in the dorsal raphe have been implicated in the pathophysiology of depression, particularly with respect to learned helplessness. This phenomenon is related to behavioral deficits in coping with stressful situations, which is displayed as a consequence of prior exposure to uncontrollable stress.30 In rats, these neurons are highly activated by aversive uncontrollable events leading to the development of depression-like behaviors.19, 30 We found that when exposed to an unknown male, female partner of either peripubertally stressed males or their male offspring displayed an increased activity in DRN serotonergic cells. This result is consistent with a recent report showing increased gene expression of two 5-HT-related transcription factors in the DRN 5-HT neurons of women, but not men, with major depressive disorder.31 Importantly, in our rat study, activation of 5-HT neurons was observed in the dorsal and caudal parts of the DR, regions that target limbic structures, such as hippocampus, septum and medial prefrontal cortex32 where the serotonergic system is implicated in the regulation of anxiety.33 A question that arises from these findings is whether these alterations in serotonergic reactivity are linked to the described blunting in the morning corticosterone levels and the emotional changes. Although further studies are needed to provide a mechanistic response, support for this hypothesis is suggested by a strong evidence of the existence of a close bidirectional relationship between the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the serotonergic system.34, 35 More specifically, a link between impaired glucocorticoid function and hyperresponsiveness of the raphe 5-HT system following stress has been reported in genetically modified animals that showed increased reactivity to stress.36

The high aggression levels displayed by peripubertally stressed males against their female partners is consistent with human data, indicating that exposure to childhood trauma is especially high in perpetrators of domestic violence.4, 5 The potential of early life stress (maternal separation from the pups during the early postnatal period) to induce increased inter-male aggression in rats has been previously reported.37 To our knowledge, this is the first report showing that early life stress in rats can also lead to increased aggression toward female partners. Thus, our data support the view that biological factors triggered by adverse juvenile experiences can have a central role in the maturation of male perpetrators of intimate domestic violence during adulthood and illustrate the negative consequences experienced by their female partners.

Although most studies of the intergenerational transmission of family violence in humans have focused on the individual's lifespan from victimization to perpetration, there are historical and epidemiological accounts that the transmission of violence can occur across several generations.38, 39 This concept is further supported by increased cases of intimate partner violence among children of surviving victims of domestic violence.40 Thus, our data showing increased aggressive behavior toward females in the peripubertally stressed rats’ offspring fits with the human literature, illustrating the transmission of violence toward females across generations and further support a role for biological factors in this phenomenon.

The observation that male offspring displaying increased aggression toward their female partners is consistent with emerging evidence supporting the occurrence of paternal effects in offspring in the absence of postnatal father–offspring interaction.41 Multiple mechanisms may be driving these paternal effects,41 including the impact of gestational42 or early postnatal stress associated with maternal anxiety,43 changes in the quality of the maternal care44 [however, it is important to note that no differences in maternal care were observed among the offspring of the control and peripubertally stressed males used in the F1 study (Figure 4 and Supplementary Figure S2)] or epigenetic factors transmitted through the germline.45 Rodent studies have presented evidence for the transmission of behavioral traits across generations through both maternal behavioral effects44, 46 and through paternal mechanisms involving epigenetic transmission through the germline47, 48 or through other paternally-induced maternal adaptations.49

Altogether, our results highlight a key role for biological factors in the link between juvenile exposure to trauma and the ensuing development of pathological aggression toward intimate partners. These findings, along with the assemblage of physiological and behavioral symptoms observed in females cohabiting with either the peripubertally stressed males or with their male offspring, confirm the detrimental impact of these close interactions on females’ indexes of physical and mental health. Our results also indicate that this phenotype can be transmitted to the next generation in the absence of father–offspring postnatal interaction. Although extrapolation from animals to humans should always be made with caution, our work strongly supports the need to extend the currently dominant psychosocial explanations of the traumatic stress-induced violence cycle to include biological factors. In addition to opening new perspectives for prevention and intervention programs addressing the inter- and trans-generational transmission of intimate partner violence, our findings urge the development of research efforts aimed at identifying the precise biological mechanisms driving these effects.